Friday, October 28th, 2011
Annotated Bibliography (1)
Gazzaniga, Michael S., Richard B. Ivry, and George R. Mangun Cognitive Neuroscience: The Biology of the Mind 3rd Edition. New York: W.W. Norton & Company, 2009
Learning is the process of acquiring new information and it results in memory. Learning/memory is generally divided into 3 stages: A) Encoding – the process in which new information enters to be stored, two steps: acquisition and consolidation. Acquisition registers inputs of sensory information/sensory analysis; consolidation “creates a stronger representation over time” (313) B) Storage – a ‘permanent’ record of the information, C) Retrieval – “utilizes stored information to created a conscious representation or to execute a learned behavior such as a motor act” (313). Many neurologists are fond of explaining remembering as “mental time travel” – that is to say, the past is experienced in the present. But not all memories are created equal. Memory is usually distinguished as sensory memory, short-term memory or long-term memory. Sensory memory has a lifetime of milliseconds to seconds, short-term memory lasts seconds to minutes, and long-term memory lasts days to years. In 1968 cognitive psychologists Richard Atkinson and Richard Shiffrin created one of the first memory models. In their modal model, information is first stored in sensory memory and “items selected by attentional processes can move into short-term storage” (315) and then “once in short-term memory, if the item is rehearsed it can be moved into long-term memory” (315). The Akinson and Shiffrin modal model suggests, “at each stage, information can be lost by decay (information degrades and is lost over time), interference (new information displaces old information), or a combination of both” (315). This model has been debated and expanded upon—particularly whether a long-term memory must first be a short-term memory—or if a short-term memory is simply a temporary activation of the long-term mechanisms
Later findings supported the idea that “short-term memory might not be required in order to form long-term memories” and that sensory memories can be registered immediately as long-term memories (316). A new theory emerged that “short-term memory is supported by activation of the same neural networks that are involved in storing long-term memory, but not in precisely the same fashion or to the same extent” (317). The concept of working memory was developed to extend the concept of short-term memory.
Working-memory “represents a limited-capacity store for retaining information over the short term (maintenance) and for performing mental operations on the contents of this store (manipulation)” (317). Working memory contains “information that can be acted on and processed, not merely maintained by rehearsal, although such maintenance is one aspect of working memory” (317). There are 2 kinds of long-term memory – declarative (explicit) and nondeclarative (implicit). Declarative memory “is knowledge to which we have conscious access, including personal and world knowledge” and nondeclarative memory is “knowledge to which we have no conscious access, such as motor and cognitive skills (procedural knowledge), perceptual priming, and simple learned behaviors that derive from conditioning, habituation, or sensitization” (321). Declarative memory includes episodic and semantic memory; what we recall about our own lives and factual information. Nondeclarative memory includes procedural memory (learning to ride a bike/read), perceptual representation system (perceptual priming), classical conditioning (conditioned responses between two stimuli) and non-associative learning (habituation, sensitization). The medial temporal area includes the amygdala, the hippocampus, the entorhinal cotex and the sourrounding parahippocampal and perirhinal cortical areas. The hippocampus is crucial in forming long-term memories—and in recalling them. Functional-MRI studies have shown the it is active when new information is encoded (subjects are typically given items and asked to remember them while their neural activity is measured by an fMRI) and later they are asked to remember the items. The hippocampus was active during the encoding and the remembering.
When our memory fails, we usually forget—“sometimes, however, something more surprising occurs: We remember events that never happened” (350) Henry Roediger and Kathleen McDermott at Washington University presented subjects with a list of words “(e.g., thread, pin, eye, sewing, sharp, point, haystack, pain, injection, ect)”, “in which all the words are highly associated to a word that is not presented” in the case above – needle. “When subjects are asked subsequently to recall or recognize the words on the list, they show a strong tendency to falsely remember the associated word that was not presented” (350). The “”memory illusion is so powerful that “participants are willing to claim that they vividly remember seeing the non-presented critical word in the study list” (350). Yet “when participants are interrogated carefully about the conscious experience associated with remembering items from the list (true items) and the critical non-preented words (false items), they tend to rate true items higher than false items in terms of sensory details (Mather et al., 1997; K.A. Normal & Schacter, 1997)” (350). We believe in illusory recollections but are able to distinguish them from genuine recollections in terms of sensory detail.
There is a dissociation between two of the medial temporal lobe regions. In the hippocampus bilaterally, “false items elicited more neural activity than did new items; whereas in the left parahippocampul gyrus—a region surrounding the hippocampus—false items elicited about the same amount of activity as new items and significantly less activity than true items” (350). The hippocampus responses in more or less the same way regardless of whether or not the item is true or false. Yet the parahippocampal gyrus responded more strongly to the true items. True and false items are similar when it comes to semantic content but differ when it comes to sensory content. The hippocampus recalls semantic information and the parahippocampal gyrus recals sensory information.
There is a distinction between recollection and familiarity in the retrieval of long-term memories—one for “recognition based on the recollection of episodic (source) information involving the hippocampus and posteriror parahippocampal cortex, and the other for supporting familiarity-based recognition memory in the entorhinal cotex” (349). The hippocampus plays a role in encoding and retrieving episodic memories that are recollected. The areas outside the hippocampus (particularly the entorhinal cortex) support recognition based on familiarity” (351). This is because “the neocortex gives different types of information to different subregions of the medial temporal lobe. When we process “what” an item is, it enters the entrohinal cotex while information about “where” something is located enters a different part of the entorhinal cortex” (352)
I will need to understand the structural workings of memory, although I am not sure what aspects I will refer to specifically. I will be writing about the differences between Gretta’s memories of Michael Furey (invoked by sensory input) and Gabriel’s memories of Gretta. I will also be talking about the national memory of Ireland—not in terms of a structural neurology of memory, but rather by paralleling phenomena in individual memory with phenomena in national memory, particularly how memory relates to identity.
Henigan, Julie “’The Old Irish Tonality’: Folksong as Emotional Catalyst in ‘The Dead’” New Hibernia Review, Vol. 11, No. 4 (2007) pp. 136-148
Although most critics address why the song “The Lass of Aughrim” was used in the story. Most critics, Henigan claims, have examined “the lyrics, the narrative and…[connections] between its images or events and those of the story (136). She refers to the song, backed by another critic, Séamus Reilly, that the song triggers “the moment of remembering Joyce wishes to depiect” (Reilly). The reason the story is used, according to Henigan, lies “less in the ballad’s lyrics than in the song’s overall impact, both lyrically and musically, the emotion it evokes by virtues of the qualities inherent in traditional song itself” (141). She also points to biographical information from Joyce’s life and his connection to the song. Since I will be looking at music in general and its role in memory, Henigan’s investigation of the song, not as a poem, but as music, will be relevant because I can point to its overall qualities.
Kelleher, John “Irish History and Mythology in James Joyce’s ‘The Dead’” The Review of Politics Vol. 27 No. 3 (Jul. 1965) pp. 414-433
Kelleher claims that there are 3 subsidiary levels in the story which “provide an eerie, ominous, reverberation as between present and past, passing life and all-devouring death, mundane reality and myth” (418). These levels are: 1) a symbolic level (Mr. Browne = death), 2) reference to the historical time period, early 19th C Catholic Dublin, 3) References to the Old Irish Saga ‘The Destruction of Da Derga’s Hostel’. Although Kelleher gives several insights into the text’s symbolic and historical levels, he spends most of the essay on the last and weakest level. I can use this essay in my own research because Kelleher discusses a number of different aspects of the story. I can use his various claims either as support for my own claims or as a platform from which to disagree or partially disagree. He convincingly argues that consideration should be given to the opinions of Joyce’s brother, Stanislaus Joyce, on The Dead in his book Recollections of James Joyce.K elleher claims that “The Dead” is not primarily a ghost story, but rather naturalistic. He claims that “there is no reason to assume that Michael Furey is actually a ghost” but is instead only a memory. I argue that the role of memory is central to the story. Kelleher disagrees with Ellmann’s view that the famous ‘westward journey’ line refers to a trip westward to Galway. He claims that it in fact refers to death. I believe that the line is purposefully ambiguous and could easily refer to both.
Lehrer, Jonah Proust was a Neuroscientist First Mariner Books: Boston (2008)
Lehrer investigates the cross section of science and art by looking at what certain artists have told us about human thought and how their claims correlate to those of contemporary neuroscientists. Lehrer looks at the influence of philosopher Henri Bergson on Marcel Proust and Proust’s ability to examine his own remembering process. Proust explores how time “mutates” memory (81). Proust believed that our recollections are largely false. Lehrer writes that “in order to investigate the reality of our past, in order to understand memory as we actually experience it, scientists needed to confront the specter of memory’s lie” as Proust did (83). Lehrer references Santiago Ramon y Cajal who claimed that memories exist as “subtle shifts in the strength of synapses, which makes it easier for neurons to communicate with one another” (84). This allows us to take in a stimuli, process it, and relate it to a previous intake of the stimuli which in turn relates it to a memory. He also references a study by Karim Nader, Glenn Shafe, and Joseph LeDoux at NYU in 2000, who claim that the act of remembering also changes the person, and “a memory is only as real as the last time you remembered it. The more you remember something, the less accurate the memory becomes” (85). The experiment revealed memory “as a ceaseless process, not a repository of inert information…any time we remember anything, the neuronal structure of the memory is delicately transformed, a process called reconsolidation” (85). He also references a study by Dr. Kausik Si, in the journal Cell that investigates the structural level of memories. I will be formatting my paper in a similar way and will be reading the three studies on memory that Lehrer uses an summarizing them separately.
Roos, Bonnie “James Joyce’s ‘The Dead’ and Bret Harte’s Gabriel Conroy: The Nature of the Feast” The Yale Journal of Criticism Vol. 15 No. 1 (Spring 2002) pp. 99-126
Roos parallels ‘The Dead’ with Dante’s story of Ugolino and with Bret Harte’s novel Gabriel Conroy while focusing on the role and nature of of the feast. Roos argues that Joyce’s writing, or lack of writing, about the Famine is more complicated than previous criticism has understood and that “The Dead” is a critique of Ireland’s national and political complicity in its colonization. Roos examines the theme of cannibalism—how it relates to “the Morkan feast”, linked with its opposite, starvation—and to Ireland’s colonization. Roos writes that Joyce was writing, in part, about the famine and its aftermath. Roos writes that in Ireland, the population was incredibly low, both due to the famine and the large number of people, especially men, who were leaving. Gabriel’s speech failed, according to Roos, in part because Gabriel’s role as a leader was false; his position in life is due in large part to the fact that other men have died or left. His speech then “highlights an Irish nationalism that he does not feel, articulated in language that is not Irish, and emphasizes an Irish self-sacrificing hospitality which he does not willingly abide.” Gabriel “covers up the truth of Irish experience with sentimental ideals”. Many of Roos’ points, especially about the role of what is absent yet invoked throughout the story will relate to what I plan to write relating to identity and ghosts.
Whalen, Kevin “The Memories of ‘The Dead’” The Yale Journal of Criticism Vol. 15 No. 1 (Spring 2002) pp.59-97
Whelan offers an “excavation of the historical layers—biographical, literary, historical, geographical, musical” of The Dead, specifically looking at the story’s references to the Famine. He claims that post-Famine Ireland gravitated towards Modernist writing because it was a country coping with an identity crisis and already conscious of language. Whalen spends most of the essay exploring Ireland’s past and its influence on writers of Joyce’s time, often focusing specifically on The Dead. ‘The Irish past, like Michael Furey in ‘The Dead’,” he writes, ‘can only return to the present as an absence: the Irish language, love, a national community have all been consigned to the spectral” (Whalen, 2002 p. 66). For Whalen, Michael Furey can be seen as a “symbolic of a vibrant, passionate life which has vanished” from Ireland (Whalen, p. 70). Although Whalen does an excellent job of providing a historical and biographical context for “The Dead”, viewing it through several lenses, and not just a historical one, will advance literary knowledge about narrative technique. Literary critics have yet to explore “The Dead” through a cognitive lens, but the narrative technique explores and plays with human thought.